Âé¶¹ÒùÔº


This article has been reviewed according to Science X's and . have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

Bonobos combine calls in similar ways to human language, study finds

Bonobos combine calls in similar ways to human language
Mia, a young bonobo female from the Fekako community, vocalizing in response to distant group members. Credit: Martin Surbeck, Kokolopori Bonobo Research Project

Bonobos—our closest living relatives—create complex and meaningful combinations of calls resembling the word combinations of humans.

A study, conducted by researchers at the University of Zurich and Harvard University, challenges long-held assumptions about what makes unique and suggests that key aspects of language are evolutionarily ancient. The research is in the journal Science.

The study has investigated the vocal behavior of wild bonobos in the Kokolopori Community Reserve (Democratic Republic of Congo). Researchers at the University of Zurich and Harvard University used novel methods borrowed from linguistics to demonstrate for the first time that, similarly to human language, bonobo vocal communication relies extensively on compositionality.

Compositionality is the capacity to combine meaningful words into phrases whose meaning is related to the meaning of the words and the way they are combined. In more trivial compositionality, the meaning of the combination is the addition of its parts: for example, "blond dancer" refers to a person who is both blond and a dancer.

However, in more complex, nontrivial compositionality, one part of the combination modifies the other. For example, "bad dancer" does not refer to a bad person who is also a dancer: "bad" in this case does not have an independent meaning but complements "dancer."

A bonobo emits a subtle peep before the whistle, to denote tensed social situations. (Here, the bonobo is performing a display in front of the other group members by dragging a branch). Credit: Mélissa Berthet

A bonobo dictionary

In a first step, the researchers applied a method developed by linguists to quantify the meaning of human words. "This allowed us to create a bonobo dictionary of sorts—a complete list of bonobo calls and their meaning," says Mélissa Berthet, a postdoctoral researcher at the Department of Evolutionary Anthropology of UZH and lead researcher of the study.

"This represents an important step towards understanding the communication of other species, as it is the first time that we have determined the meaning of calls across the whole vocal repertoire of an animal."

A bonobo whistling in the forest, to coordinate group movements over larger distances. Credit: Mélissa Berthet

Compositionality is not unique to humans

After determining the meaning of single bonobo vocalizations, the researchers then moved on to investigating call combinations, using another approach borrowed from linguistics.

"With our approach, we were able to quantify how the meaning of bonobo single calls and call combinations relate to each other," says Simon Townsend, UZH Professor and senior author of the study. The researchers found numerous call combinations whose meaning was related to the meaning of their single parts, a key hallmark of compositionality.

Furthermore, some of the call combinations bore a striking resemblance to the more complex nontrivial compositional structures in human language.

"This suggests that the capacity to combine call types in complex ways is not as unique to humans as we once thought," says Berthet.

  • Bonobos combine calls in similar ways to human language
    Tupac, a young male bonobo scratching its head. Credit: Lukas Bierhoff, Kokolopori Bonobo Research Project
  • Bonobos combine calls in similar ways to human language
    Olive, a fist time bonobo mother from the Ekalakala community, vocalizing toward distant group members. Credit: Lukas Bierhoff, Kokolopori Bonobo Research Project

Older than previously thought

An important implication of this research is the potential light it sheds on the evolutionary roots of language's compositional nature.

"Since humans and bonobos had a approximately 7 to 13 million years ago, they share many traits by descent, and it appears that compositionality is likely one of them," says Harvard Professor Martin Surbeck, co-author of the study.

"Our study therefore suggests that our ancestors already extensively used compositionality at least 7 million years ago, if not more," adds Simon Townsend.

The findings also indicate that the ability to construct complex meanings from smaller vocal units existed long before human language emerged, and that vocal communication shares more similarities with human language than previously thought.

More information: M. Berthet, Extensive compositionality in the vocal system of bonobos, Science (2025). .

Journal information: Science

Provided by University of Zurich

Citation: Bonobos combine calls in similar ways to human language, study finds (2025, April 3) retrieved 28 April 2025 from /news/2025-04-bonobos-combine-similar-ways-human.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Chimpanzees combine calls to communicate new meaning

56 shares

Feedback to editors